Survival of the Friendliest

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
Survival of the Friendliest: Understanding Our Origins and Rediscovering Our Common Humanity
US first edition cover
AuthorBrian Hare
Vanessa Woods
Cover artistAbbey Lossing (illustration)
Greg Mollica (design)
CountryUnited States
LanguageEnglish
PublisherRandom House
Publication date
July 14, 2020
Media typePrint (hardcover)
Pages304
ISBN978-0-399-59066-5
OCLC1124903559
155.7
LC ClassBF698.95 H37 2020

Survival of the Friendliest: Understanding Our Origins and Rediscovering Our Common Humanity[1] is a book by anthropologist Brian Hare and writer Vanessa Woods, first published in 2020, based on Hare's research hypothesis of human self-domestication.[2] The main thesis of the book is that late in human evolution Homo sapiens underwent a process of extreme selection for friendliness that led to the self-domestication syndrome, as seen in other animals. The self-domestication syndrome led to a series of cognitive changes that allowed modern humans to out compete other species of humans in the Pleistocene, including Neanderthals, and become the most successful mammal on the planet. Hare and Woods argue that self-domestication is an ongoing process that continues today.

Summary[edit]

There are many theories on why modern humans became so successful while other human species went extinct, which generally revolve around humans becoming more intelligent which led to improvements in weapons and technology. Hare and Woods argue that instead, humans underwent extreme selection for prosociality, and that cognitive changes occurred by accident.

Domestication syndrome[edit]

Domestication is a process of human induced artificial selection that causes marked changes in an animal compared to their wild relatives. Collectively called the domestication syndrome, these changes include physiology (increases in serotonin, oxytocin), morphology (skull shape and size, tooth size, floppy ears, curly tails, star mutations), behavior (reproductive cycle, juvenile behavior), and social cognition (increase in cooperative communicative abilities). Different domesticated animals have different combinations of these changes. Until recently, it was poorly understood why different species of domesticated animals developed which traits.[3][4][5] However, one trait that all domesticated animals share is a reduction in aggression compared to their wild relatives.

Pioneering experiments where Siberian foxes were experimentally domesticated demonstrated that domestication is directly caused by intense selection against aggression.[6] Over 50 generations, breeding only the friendliest, non-aggressive foxes, whose fear towards humans was replaced by attraction, led to a cascade of the physiological, morphological, behavioral, and cognitive changes apparent in other domesticated animals.[7]

Self-domestication[edit]

The process of self-domestication is similar, but instead of humans actively selecting against aggression, natural selection favors the process instead. As an example, in our closest living relatives, bonobos, self domestication can occur entirely without human intervention. Compared to our other closest living relatives, chimpanzees, bonobos are much less aggressive, and have never been observed to kill their group members or attack neighboring groups. Recent work shows that the friendliest bonobo male is more reproductively successful than the most despotic chimpanzee. Thus natural selection could favor reduced aggression in bonobos, which would have led to the domestication syndrome. Indeed, compared to chimpanzees, bonobos have changes to their morphology, (reduced cranial size, canine dimorphism, and depigmentation of the lips and tail tufts), physiology (changes to serotonin receptors and testosterone response), behavior (more juvenilized socio sexual behavior and play) and social cognition (increase in cooperative abilities).[8]

Human self-domestication[edit]

If self domestication could occur in bonobos, then it may also be possible in our own species. Hare and Woods propose that natural selection favored increased in-group prosociality over aggression in late human evolution. As a by-product of this selection, humans are predicted to show traits of the domestication syndrome observed in other domestic animals, including early-emerging cooperative communicative abilities.

Drawing on comparative, developmental, fossil, and neurobiological evidence, Hare and Woods propose that late human evolution was dominated by selection for intragroup prosociality over aggression. As a result, modern humans possess traits consistent with the syndrome associated with domestication in other animals. Increases in cognition, particularly related to cooperative communicative abilities, occurred by accident.

Reception[edit]

Survival of the Friendliest was well covered in the press,[9][10][11][12] but there were popular and academic critiques.

Carel Van Shaik wrote: 'A lack of empirical studies on evolutionary rates and variation thwarts meaningful comparison with domestication.'[13]

Legal scholar Cass Sunstein wrote: 'Hare and Woods make it plausible to think that an underlying propensity of Homo Sapiens—to divide the world into insiders and outsiders—is causing a great deal of contemporary turmoil. But the underlying mechanisms are numerous, and evolutionary explanations are hardly sufficient. In the United States, for example, party antagonisms are much greater now than they were forty years ago; Homo Sapiens has not changed much in that time',[14] although later in the review Sunstein conceded: 'Homo Sapiens triumphed because of our capacity to cooperate with one another. In a challenging time, that is an inspiring message—and it suggests, in the strongest possible terms, that this is a capacity to cultivate.'[14]

Conservative commenters such as Pat Gray have criticized 'their theory they are altering now to fit their sensibilities, and their cry closets'.[15]

References[edit]

  1. ^ Hare, Brian, 1976- author. (14 July 2020). Survival of the friendliest : understanding our origins and rediscovering our common humanity. Random House Publishing. ISBN 978-0-399-59067-2. OCLC 1149345326. {{cite book}}: |last= has generic name (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) CS1 maint: numeric names: authors list (link)
  2. ^ Hare, Brian (2017-01-03). "Survival of the Friendliest:Homo sapiensEvolved via Selection for Prosociality". Annual Review of Psychology. 68 (1): 155–186. doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-010416-044201. ISSN 0066-4308. PMID 27732802. S2CID 3387266.
  3. ^ Diamond, Jared (2002). "Evolution, consequences and future of plant and animal domestication". Nature. 418 (6898): 700–707. Bibcode:2002Natur.418..700D. doi:10.1038/nature01019. ISSN 1476-4687. PMID 12167878. S2CID 205209520.
  4. ^ Hemmer, H. (1990-07-27). Domestication: The Decline of Environmental Appreciation. Cambridge University Press. ISBN 978-0-521-34178-3.
  5. ^ Tchernov, Eitan; Horwitz, Liora Kolska (1991). "Body size diminution under domestication: Unconscious selection in primeval domesticates". Journal of Anthropological Archaeology. 10 (1): 54–75. doi:10.1016/0278-4165(91)90021-o. ISSN 0278-4165.
  6. ^ Trut, Lyudmila N. (1999). "Early Canid Domestication: The Farm-Fox Experiment: Foxes bred for tamability in a 40-year experiment exhibit remarkable transformations that suggest an interplay between behavioral genetics and development". American Scientist. 87 (2): 160–169. doi:10.1511/1999.2.160. ISSN 0003-0996. JSTOR 27857815.
  7. ^ Hare, Brian; Plyusnina, Irene; Ignacio, Natalie; Schepina, Olesya; Stepika, Anna; Wrangham, Richard; Trut, Lyudmila (2005). "Social Cognitive Evolution in Captive Foxes Is a Correlated By-Product of Experimental Domestication". Current Biology. 15 (3): 226–230. Bibcode:2005CBio...15..226H. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2005.01.040. ISSN 0960-9822. PMID 15694305. S2CID 1592440.
  8. ^ Hare, Brian; Wobber, Victoria; Wrangham, Richard (2012-03-01). "The self-domestication hypothesis: evolution of bonobo psychology is due to selection against aggression". Animal Behaviour. 83 (3): 573–585. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.12.007. ISSN 0003-3472. S2CID 3415520.
  9. ^ Why it actually might be 'survival of the friendliest', 30 September 2020, archived from the original on October 24, 2020, retrieved 2020-10-07
  10. ^ Sloat, Sarah. "One advantageous way humans are more like dogs than Neanderthals". Inverse. Retrieved 2020-10-07.
  11. ^ Cimons, Marlene. "'Friendliest,' not fittest, is key to evolutionary survival, scientists argue in book". Washington Post. ISSN 0190-8286. Retrieved 2020-10-07.
  12. ^ "Why our understanding of 'Survival of the Fittest' is wrong". BBC Reel. Retrieved 2020-10-07.
  13. ^ Sánchez-Villagra, Marcelo R.; Schaik, Carel P. van (2019). "Evaluating the self-domestication hypothesis of human evolution". Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews. 28 (3): 133–143. doi:10.1002/evan.21777. ISSN 1520-6505. PMID 30938920. S2CID 91189857.
  14. ^ a b Sunstein, Cass R. (2020-05-29). "The triumph of the friendly: A review of Brian Hare and Vanessa Woods, survival of the friendliest". Journal of Bioeconomics. 22 (2): 131–135. doi:10.1007/s10818-020-09298-1. ISSN 1387-6996. S2CID 219763857.
  15. ^ Gray, Pat (2020). "It is No Longer Survival of the Fittest. Now It's Survival of the Friendliest?". YouTube.